Use this URL to cite or link to this record in EThOS: http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.731998
Title: The feeding behaviour of the oystercatcher, Haematopus ostralegus
Author: Norton-Griffiths, M.
Awarding Body: University of Oxford
Current Institution: University of Oxford
Date of Award: 1968
Availability of Full Text:
Access from EThOS:
Full text unavailable from EThOS. Please try the link below.
Access from Institution:
Abstract:
In this study of the functional aspects of a complex behavioural system I have looked at three different aspects of the feeding behaviour of the Oystercatcher. I started by analysing the skilled feeding techniques of the adults, but I soon became interested in the development of these techniques in the young, and in the parental feeding system which is surprisingly different from that of other waders. It forms the 'buffer' between the adults and the young and allows the young to acquire the specialised feeding techniques and skills of their parents. Part I I discuss the parental feeding system first because the analysis has focused on a slightly different aspect of the feeding behaviour, namely the way in which the amount of food brought to the young by the parents is precisely controlled by the behaviour of the young. The parental feeding system has two basic characteristics: (a) the parents bring food items to the young and present them, and the young take the food from the parent's bill; and (b) the young accompany their parents out onto the feeding grounds and are fed by them in long 'feeding trains'. The seasonal development of parental feeding starts with the auditory and tactile interactions between the incubating parent and the hatching eggs. Both parents and young-in-egg become more responsive to each other as hatching proceeds, and both show a marked selective responsiveness. These pre-hatch interactions lead to the transition from incubation to brooding behaviour in the parent, and finally result in the onset of parental feeding. There is the additional possibility that the young learn to respond to the call of their parents while still in the egg. I then discuss the factors which control the amount of food brought by the parents to the young. In the active phases of the daily feeding cycle, each parent brings food items to the young at random moments in time. Once the food has been presented, the subsequent behaviour of the parent depends upon the speed with which the young seizes and devours the food. If the young takes the food within a certain time (the Waiting-Time-Threshold) the parent will immediately search for and present more food, and it will continue to do this for as long as the young takes the food within the threshold time. In this way the long 'feeding trains' are initiated and maintained. However, the young can also initiate a feeding train by approaching a parent who is feeding. The feeding train is broken off by the parent as soon as the young fails to react within the threshold time. This self-regulating social system is based on complex and highly specialised interactions between the parents and the young. As the young grow older the organisation of parental feeding changes drastically. The parents initiate fewer feeding trains, they carry less food to the young and, instead of removing the flesh from prey and then presenting it to the young, they begin to leave the prey for the young to open up and clean out. The young however play a more and more important role in the initiation and maintenance of the feeding trains. This switch of control to the young eventually leads to the breakdown of parental feeding, for, as the young learn to feed themselves, they stop eliciting parental feeding. The consequence of this system is that in areas where the young merely have to acquire the simple skills of probing for worms, parental feeding stops at about 6–7 weeks. However, in areas where the young are learning the more difficult skills for dealing with molluscs and crustacea, parental feeding continues for 12–20 weeks, and in extreme cases for as long as 43 weeks. Part II and III I here describe the techniques employed by adult Oystercatchers in dealing with Mytilus, Cardium, Scrobicularia, Tapes, Littorina, Patella and Carcinus. Each of these techniques consists of a sequence of discrete motor patterns of which each has a different form and a different function. To take the feeding techniques on Mytilus as an example: either the Stab (a quick downwards lunge of the bill through the gaping valves) or Hammering (a series of hard blows aimed at the shell) serve to gain initial energy into the valve; Levering (side-to-side movements of the bill) and Prising (forcibly opening the mandibles) serve to force the valves wide apart; and Chiseling (rapid to-and-fro movements of the bill) serves to cut the flesh loose from the valves. Each motor pattern is precisely oriented for maximum effect: the Stab is aimed between the posterior valve margins so that it severs the strong posterior adductor muscle when the bill enters (this disabling the mussel); the Hammering attack (for which the mussel is first oriented so that the flat ventral surface is uppermost) is aimed at this area which I have shown to be by far the weakest part of the shell; and the Chiseling movements are aimed at the largest attachments between the flesh and the shell, the smaller ones being broken when the main mass of the flesh is shaken free. I found that these motor patterns are applied to all the prey species mentioned, and that they have always the same general function. However, in each case the orientation of the motor patterns are specifically adjusted to the anatomy of the particular prey. For example, the Hammering attack is direct at the ventral valve margins of Mytilus; at the posterior, ventral or anterior valve margins of Cardium (all these valve margins are equally friable); at the lateral walls of Patella (which are most easily dislodged by a blow from this direction); and at the mouth of Carcinus (which kills the crab as well as making a hole in it.) I then analyse the organisation and control of the sequence and duration of the motor pattern. Firstly, each motor pattern is elicited by a different external stimulus. Second, the duration of the motor patterns is likewise under continuous external control; for example, the duration of Levering in Cardium depends solely upon the size of the prey, i.e. in functional terms, to the amount of Levering that is needed to force the valves apart. Thirdly, the sequence in which the motor patterns occur are seen to be simple chain responses in which the performance of one motor pattern brings about the external stimuli necessary to elicit the next. It is this total dependence on external control which imparts a remarkable flexibility, and thus efficiency and economy of effort to the techniques, for each motor pattern is performed only if and when necessary, and only for as long as is necessary. The skill, efficiency and versatility of the adult techniques are due to two basic factors: firstly, a basic repertoire of motor patterns each adapted to a very generalised function; and secondly, their dependence on external stimuli, which allows (a) the precise adjustment of the orientation of each motor pattern to the anatomy of any one prey, and (b) flexibility in the initiation, duration and sequential organisation of the motor patterns. Very surprisingly, there is good evidence that individual Oystercatchers specialise not only on one particular prey but even on one particular technique of catching it. For example, the breeding population of Ravenglass (in Cumberland) is made up of individuals who eat crabs. Furthermore, in each breeding pair both partners have the same specialisation. There are a number of both casual and functional reasons for this specialisation, most of which are bound up with the development of the techniques in the young Oystercatchers. Part IV The starting point for my study of the development of the feeding sills were the observations that the young of mussel-Stabbing parents, mussel-Hammering parents and crab-eating parents all develop the same feeding skills and techniques as their parents. I therefore cross-fostered the eggs of individually known pairs with different specialisations. In every case the young developed the specialisations of their foster parents. This meant that the young had the potential to develop a number of different techniques but that some environmental factor was channelling the developmental system, so that only one technique developed. This led to a study of (a) the basis of the flexibility in the developmental system, and (b) the ways in which the channelling was brought about so that only one technique developed, and I have tried to analyse these from both a functional and a causal viewpoint.
Supervisor: Not available Sponsor: Not available
Qualification Name: Thesis (Ph.D.) Qualification Level: Doctoral
EThOS ID: uk.bl.ethos.731998  DOI: Not available
Share: